• Thursday, May 3rd 2018 at 15:00 - 16:00 UK (Other timezones)
  • General participation info   |   Participate online   |   + Phone in United States (Toll Free): 1 877 309 2073 United States: +1 (571) 317-3129 Australia (Toll Free): 1 800 193 385 Australia: +61 2 8355 1020 Austria (Toll Free): 0 800 202148 Belgium (Toll Free): 0 800 78884 Canada (Toll Free): 1 888 455 1389 Denmark (Toll Free): 8090 1924 France (Toll Free): 0 805 541 047 Germany (Toll Free): 0 800 184 4222 Greece (Toll Free): 00 800 4414 3838 Hungary (Toll Free): (06) 80 986 255 Iceland (Toll Free): 800 9869 Ireland (Toll Free): 1 800 946 538 Israel (Toll Free): 1 809 454 830 Italy (Toll Free): 800 793887 Japan (Toll Free): 0 120 663 800 Luxembourg (Toll Free): 800 22104 Netherlands (Toll Free): 0 800 020 0182 New Zealand (Toll Free): 0 800 47 0011 Norway (Toll Free): 800 69 046 Poland (Toll Free): 00 800 1213979 Portugal (Toll Free): 800 819 575 Spain (Toll Free): 800 900 582 Sweden (Toll Free): 0 200 330 905 Switzerland (Toll Free): 0 800 740 393 United Kingdom (Toll Free): 0 800 169 0432 Access Code: 731-636-357

If humans relied solely on instrumental control to select actions, then choices would be based on whichever action maximized advantageous outcomes. However, prior research has shown that this is not the case and, instead, given a particular context, some actions are easier to select than others. This suggests a decision controller, referred to as Pavlovian control, that rigidly selects actions in the presence of certain stimuli regardless of whether those actions maximize expected value. For example, research has shown the presence of Pavlovian biases for reward and punishment, such that a potential reward elicits action (i.e. Go) and punishment elicits action inhibition (i.e. NoGo). We extended this general framework to the context of escape, where, during an ongoing aversive state, actions are selected to escape the aversive state. We show that escape is also associated with a Pavlovian bias for action and replicate prior work showing that avoiding a punishment is associated with action inhibition. We developed a model integrating reinforcement learning and drift-diffusion models that allowed us to capture both choice and reaction time.  We tested multiple instantiations of the Pavlovian bias and found that, compared with avoid, escape pushed the DDM starting point closer to a Go response. We then tested this approach in a sample of suicidal participants. Long-standing theoretical and anecdotal accounts of suicide conceptualize it as a decision to escape aversive emotional states, yet almost no prior research has examined basic decision-making processes involving escape. We hypothesized that, compared to those with a psychiatric condition but without a history of suicidal thoughts, suicidal people would show a stronger Pavlovian bias to escape and results supported the hypothesis. If there is time, I will also discuss future directions for this work.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Alexander Millner, PhD
Postdoctoral Fellow
Department of Psychology
Harvard University

Alexander Millner – Pavlovian control of escape: General effects and relevance to suicidal behaviors